Select Page

Behavioral Finance 101

Behavioral finance combines psychology and economics to explain how investors act. Instead of assuming that people (e.g., investors) always make rational financial decisions, behavioral finance proponents argue that behavioral biases strongly influence financial decisions.  Unfortunately, behavioral biases can have a detrimental impact on investment results; thus, it is crucial for investors to be aware of, understand, and be able to identify the various behavioral biases that may impact their investment decisions.  In this Navigator, we discuss some of the most common biases that can hamper an investor’s ability to make rational long- term investment decisions and provide several tools to help minimize the influence of behavioral biases in financial decision making.

Behavioral Finance 101

As a rule of thumb, most economists believe that people are rational decision makers and always do what is in their economic  best  interest.  In  financial  circles, there  is  an often-told joke about two economists who come across a $20 bill on the ground.  The younger economist says, “Look, there’s a twenty-dollar bill on the ground.”  The older and wiser economist, without looking at the ground, says to him, “Impossible.  If there had been a twenty-dollar bill lying on the ground, someone would have already picked it up by now.”  In a perfect, rational, and efficient world, twenty- dollar bills never sit on the ground.

In  contrast,  behavioral  psychologists  have  demonstrated that people often make mistakes, including mistakes that are absolutely not in their best economic interest – in direct contradiction to generally accepted economic theory.

All of us make bad decisions from time to time.  Sometimes we get our facts wrong, sometimes we do not consider all the alternatives, and other times we simply react emotionally and reflexively to our circumstances.  No matter the cause, poorly thought-out decisions can have a deeply negative impact on our lives, especially when it comes to investing. Many poor financial decisions are the result of identifiable behavioral biases  that  impact our ability to think clearly and rationally.  The field of behavioral finance has arisen in recent years in an attempt to better understand these biases and their potential impacts on individuals, capital markets, and society as a whole.

The field of psychology has identified a number of behavioral biases that impact investment decisions and decision making in general, which are typically grouped into two broad categories: cognitive biases and emotional biases.  In this Navigator, we do not address every behavioral bias, but, rather, we focus on those biases which we believe can be most impactful to investors in their financial decision-making.

Cognitive Biases

Cognitive  biases  are  mental  shortcuts  that  people  take when processing information and making decisions.  These cognitive biases can lead to errors – because the brain has limited information processing capacity, and, in an attempt to simply information, information may be distorted, illogically interpreted, or inaccurately judged. Some primary examples of cognitive biases include confirmation bias, gamblers’ fallacy, negativity bias, and anchoring.

 

  • Confirmation Bias
    Confirmation  bias  is  the  tendency to  seek  out, interpret,  and  favor  evidence  or  opinions  that match our own pre-determined conclusions, while discounting the value of contradictory information.  This  bias can  be  especially  strong  in  relation to emotionally  charged  subject matters  or  deeply held beliefs.  It can also lead to the interpretation of ambiguous information as supporting one’s pre-existing beliefs.

    For  example,  consider  an  investor  who  has received a tip from a friend about a hot new stock. Confirmation bias would suggest that the investor is likely to seek out information that supports the friend’s recommendation to buy the stock rather than to seek out information that contradicts the recommendation.  In an effort to “prove” that the stock really is a great investment, the investor is likely to find a number of sources or data points that support the investment potential of the stock while disregarding or discounting potential red flags. This behavior can be dangerous, as it can cause investors to make faulty decisions that are based on poor interpretation of important information.

    In order to effectively combat confirmation bias, investors should first seek to prove the original recommendation or thesis wrong; in this case, investing cynicism provides key support in investing due diligence.  Investors should actively try to find reasons that the stock might actually be a poor investment opportunity.   If the original thesis can stand up to this critical analysis, then the investor should actually have more confidence that the stock truly represents a good investment opportunity.

     

  • Gamblers’ Fallacy
    The gamblers’ fallacy is the mistaken belief that if  something  happens  more  frequently  during  a certain period, it will happen less frequently in the future; or conversely, that if something happens less frequently during some period, it will happen more frequently in the future.  Yet, it is erroneous to believe that an independent event is more or less likely to occur because of independent events that preceded it.

    For  instance,  at  one  time  or another,  all  of  us have flipped a coin in the air several times, with the same side coming up each time, and thought, “Surely the next flip must go the other way; it’s about time.”   In this case, we have fallen victim to the gamblers’ fallacy.  In fact, even if you were to flip a coin 1,000 times and get heads each and every time (however unlikely this scenario may be), the flipping history would still have no bearing on  the  probability  of  the  outcome  of  the  next flip (assuming that the coin is evenly weighted). While it is unquestioned that we should expect to see an equal number of heads or tails over many coin flips, it does not preclude the possibility of long streaks of one outcome or the other.

    The gamblers’ fallacy is prevalent in the investing world.     It  is  common  for  investors  to  assume that  the  market  or  a  stock  will  go  down  (up) after an extended period of upward (downward) movement, simply because “it’s about time.”  But, as demonstrated many times in financial history, this logic is flawed and can lead investors to buy or sell positions with no good rationale. Of course, this is not to say that extended periods of upward or downward movement should not be considered when making investment decisions, as such movement can cause a stock to become over- or under-valued on a fundamental basis and lead to a deserved re-pricing.  But the simple fact that a stock has seen several up or down days in a row should not affect the probability of seeing a move in the opposite direction in the days ahead.

  • Negativity Bias
    The negativity bias causes investors to put more weight on bad news than on good.   The human brain is naturally wired to process negative data more quickly and more thoroughly than positive data.  Similarly, negative experiences are felt more deeply and have a far longer-lasting effect on our psychological   states   than  positive experiences.  This negative tilt, like many of our biases, has an evolutionary origin.  The  human instinct  to  put more weight on negative events than positive ones can be an effective risk management tool. It can be a strong deterrent from engaging in risky behavior or making poor choices, and it is easy to see how our ancestors might have developed such a self-preservative  tendency.  However,  the  negativity bias can have detrimental impacts on our ability to make sound investment decisions. 

    The stock market crash that occurred in conjunction with the 2008 Financial Crisis was nothing short of traumatic for millions of investors.  Many investors lost much of their life savings in the market rout, and the psychological impact of that event cannot be overstated.  Even now, nearly eight years from the  market’s nadir,  many  investors  still  remain on the sidelines; these investors are simply too paralyzed with fear to begin investing again.  This behavior is the negativity bias on full display. For many, nearly eight years of equity gains have not been enough to overcome the negative memory of 2008-2009.

    We   are   not   suggesting   that   investors   should blindly ignore negative events or make the faulty assumption that there will not be other negative events in the future.   But investors should be cognizant of their own negative bias and try to make investment decisions by assigning appropriate weights to both risk and reward.

  • Anchoring
    Anchoring is the tendency to be influenced by an initial anchor – the first piece of information that is offered when making a decision, even when the anchor is generated by an arbitrary or biased source and  so  has  no  logical  relevance  to  the  decision at  hand.    This  very  common  bias  is  particularly prevalent in investing.   Imagine an investor who buys ABC stock at $120/share.  The investor buys the stock in his IRA, so taxes have no impact on decision-making.  The stock then drops in reaction to negative news and trades down to $90/share. The investor decides to hold onto the stock until the initial purchase price (the anchor) is reached.  This decision has nothing to do with the value of the stock but only is related to an irrelevant data point to make the investor feel better about not losing money.  However, the money was lost as soon as the stock dropped to $90.   The rational investor should ingest the new information, decide what the new value for the stock would be, and then make a decision as to whether holding onto the stock continues to make logical sense.

    Investors anchor to many reference points.   A stock’s original purchase price, a high-water mark, an analyst’s fair value estimate – all of these data points can influence an investor’s decision-making, even though they do not affect market pricing.  In order to reduce the effects of anchoring, investors must remain disciplined and only consider truly relevant information when making decisions. Using a systematic and data-driven approach to making investment decisions can help investors to focus on important factors that actually drive shareholder value, rather than irrelevant noise.

Emotional Biases

A cognitive bias is the result of taking a cognitive shortcut, but an emotional bias is one that results in taking action based on feelings instead of facts.  That is, emotional biases may lead people to make poor decisions that are clouded by emotions and not based on rational judgment. Examples of emotional biases are loss aversion, overconfidence, endowment bias, and herd behavior.

  • Loss Aversion
    Loss  aversion  refers  to  the  tendency  of  people to  strongly prefer  avoiding  losses  as  opposed to acquiring  gains.    Research  suggests that  the psychological pain that we experience from losing money is roughly twice as strong as the joy that we feel when we make money, and the implications of this bias can be significant.   Humans’ aversion to loss is so powerful that it can actually lead to risk taking instead of risk aversion.To illustrate this point, imagine an investor who owns a stock that has lost a significant amount of value due to deterioration in fundamentals. Future prospects for the company look poor, and there is much uncertainty surrounding the business.  In such a case, continuing to hold the stock is risky, as there still could be meaningful additional downside in the stock.  However, many investors who find themselves in this position continue to hold the stock, for no other reason than that selling the stock would make the loss “real” in their minds rather than just a paper loss.  The prospect of having to face such a powerfully negative feeling is enough to cause many investors to actually take on more risk by continuing to hold the stock instead of simply selling the stock and moving on to better and less risky investments.Interestingly,  we  can  also  observe loss  aversion in situations where investors have actually experienced a gain on an investment. The prospect of seeing an unrealized gain on an investment dissipate can be so emotionally distressing that many investors will sell a winning investment too soon, completely ignoring the possibility that the company’s fundamentals suggest additional upside.

    Loss aversion is one of the strongest emotional biases faced because of the intensity of emotion that is felt when we experience loss.  Accordingly, we must be especially vigilant in identifying this bias. While it is impossible to entirely avoid the effects of loss aversion, we can try to minimize them by being intellectually honest with ourselves when an investment has gained or lost value and remaining focused on an investment’s fundamentals instead of how much money we might stand to gain or lose by selling it.

  • Overconfidence

    In the overconfidence bias, a person’s subjective confidence in his or her judgment, ability, or belief is  greater than  the  objective  accuracy  of  that judgment, ability, or belief.  It is the mis-calibration of subjective probabilities that results in a person having more confidence in him- or herself than the objective parameters of the situations suggest.

    People  tend  to  be  overconfident  in their  abilities in many arenas, from karaoke singing to starting a business to parallel parking.  This phenomenon has been highlighted time and again in study after study. For example, in one particular study, a group of college students were asked to rate their driving abilities as either below average, average, or above average. Statistically, self-ratings should be distributed evenly between these three categories.  However, as much as 82% of the students rated their abilities as above average, which is clearly impossible.i In a similar study, a group of entrepreneurs were asked to rate their businesses’ chances of success.  81% believed that their businesses had at least a 70% chance of success, while 33% believed that their chances of success were 100%!  In reality, the data suggest that only 25% of those businesses would exist five years later.ii

    Investing provides plenty of fertile ground for overconfidence  to  take root,  and,  consequently, this bias is very commonly observed in investors’ behavior in the capital markets. There are two sides to every trade (which makes a market!), and each side believes his or her information and/or analysis to be superior to that of the counterparty.   By definition, this must be the case or the trade would never happen.  But both parties cannot possibly be correct!  Confidence in one’s investment analysis and conclusions is what ultimately leads one to make a trade, which, in and of itself, is completely rational.  However, overconfidence can lead to making too many trades, which most certainly can be detrimental to a long-term investing strategy.  Research has shown that overconfidence leads to increased portfolio turnover rate, which is strongly correlated with sub-par returns.    Put simply, investors who are overconfident in their stock- picking abilities tend to trade more often and earn lower returns than investors who do not suffer from this bias.

    One of the most effective ways to combat the overconfidence bias is to seek out analysis that conflicts with one’s own investment theories.   By seeking   to   understand   how   one’s   conclusions might be wrong, an investor’s confidence can be tempered, resulting in a more measured approach to trading decisions.

Endowment Bias

Endowment bias suggests that people assign more value to something if they own it than if they do not.  This bias applies to a number of possessions, including homes, automobiles,    stocks,    and practically any other item.  In one experiment that was performed on a group of Cornell University students, one-half  of  the  students were given a coffee  cup,  and  the  other  one-half  were  given nothing.  Then  those students  who  had  been given coffee cups were asked at what price each of them would sell his or her cup.  The median price at which the group agreed to sell their cups was $5.25.  However, when the students who had not been given coffee cups were asked to give a price at which they would buy the cups from the other group, no one in the group was willing to pay more than $2.75. This sizeable gap seems to indicate that the selling group placed more value on the coffee cups simply because they owned the cups.iii

Early research on this topic suggested that the origin of endowment bias was actually loss aversion. Because  a  person  selling  an  item  would  feel  a sense of loss upon the sale, he or she wanted to be compensated for that painful feeling by charging a higher price, even though he or she would never pay that price to buy the item.  However, more recent studies have shown that the endowment bias is even simpler than this.iv      In a modified version of the original coffee cup experiment, it was found that buyers would pay a higher price for a coffee cup if they already owned an identical cup. This phenomenon indicates that simply owning an item causes a person to assign more value to it.

The  investment  implications  of  this  bias  should be obvious.  An investor may be disinclined to sell assets that have become fully valued because the mere act of ownership impairs the investor’s ability to properly value the asset. Holding onto assets that have become fully valued on a fundamental basis introduces incremental risk to a portfolio.  For this reason, an investor should not take a position in an investment without having a clear and disciplined exit plan.   This exit plan may change over time because of changes in fundamentals, but having a stated plan for when to sell an investment can help to offset or avoid the effects of endowment bias. Investors should note that securities are merely tradable assets, and one should not have an emotional relationship with any investable asset should it become over-valued.

  • Herd Behavior
    Herd  behavior  is  the  tendency  for  individuals to  mimic  the  actions of  a  larger  group,  even  if, individually, most people would make a different decision.  Herd  behavior  is  generally  regarded as  one  of  the  primary  causes  of  society’s  most spectacular  –  and  often spectacularly  damaging – bubbles.  A number of historical examples exist, including the infamous Dutch tulip mania of the 1630s, when the price of a single tulip bulb reached the equivalent of 10 times the annual income of a skilled craftsman! Certainly, irrational forces impact investor behavior here.  So why do humans exhibit this type of behavior?  Research has suggested two possible explanations, both with strong analogues in the animal kingdom.One reason that animals join herds is to benefit from information that other members of the herd may have, such as knowledge of food and water resources.  Humans behave similarly.  We assume that a group of investors who have started to pursue a certain strategy must know something that we do not, and we therefore join the group in order to benefit from that knowledge.  The theory is that there simply is not a way that so many people can be wrong – or so the thinking goes.  But because humans often value resources according to what they think others will pay for those same resources in the future, the lifespan for any popular strategy is inherently limited. 

    Another reason that animals join herds is to protect themselves from predators.  By being a part of a herd, an animal increases the odds of a predator choosing another member of the herd as its prey.  By essentially hiding in the herd, an animal can decrease its risk.  Investors do effectively the same thing. They hide in the herd in order to avoid the risk of being alone.  Professional investors who pursue contrarian strategies that go against conventional thinking (i.e., the herd) face reputational risk if they are wrong, while being wrong with the rest of the herd can be easily excused.  However, most great investors became so by thinking independently and pursuing strategies that actually benefited from the fact that herds are often wrong – just think of the billions that John Paulson made shorting subprime debt during the Financial Crisis.  True investment opportunity often lies in eschewing the herd and taking a contrarian stance that is based on thorough and competent analysis.

Closing Thoughts

The behavioral biases that we have outlined are powerful and prevalent, creating somewhat of a psychological minefield for investors to navigate.  It should be noted that we only touched on some of the most commonly observed biases; certainly, many other psychological investing pitfalls are out there lurking for the unaware investor.  No doubt these biases make the already challenging task of investing that much more difficult by clouding investors’ judgment and pushing them to make decisions contrary to their own long-term self-interest.  Only by being aware of behavioral biases and their nature can one hope to dull their effects.1

Finding suitable investments with attractive risk/reward characteristics is a big part of what we strive to do at Appleseed Capital, but it is far from the only activity we perform.   One of our most important responsibilities as investment advisors is helping our clients to make good financial decisions.  By providing knowledgeable, objective advice and removing the burden of trading decisions from our  clients’  shoulders,  we  try  to  help  them  avoid  many of  the  adverse  consequences  of  the  completely  normal but potentially detrimental human tendencies previously outlined.

Should you have any questions about this Navigator and the topic of behavioral biases, please reach out to your portfolio manager.

1  At Appleseed Capital, we incorporate checks and procedures into our investment process to help mitigate our own behavioral biases

For further reading on these topics, please review: 

i  Ola Svenson, “Are We All Less Risky and More Skillful Than Our Fellow Drivers?” Acta Psychologica 47(1981): 143-148

ii  Arnold C. Cooper, Carolyn Y. Woo, and William C. Dunkelberg, “Entrepreneurs’ Perceived Chances of Success,” Journal of Business Venturing 3(1988): 97-108

iii  Daniel Kahneman, Jack L. Knetsch, Richard H. Thaler, “Anomalies: The Endowment Effect, Loss Aversion, and Status  Quo  Bias,”  The  Journal  of  Economic  Perspectives 5(1991): 193-206

iv Carey K. Morewedge, Lisa L. Shu, Daniel T. Gilbert, Timothy D. Wilson, “Bad riddance or good rubbish?  Ownership and not loss aversion causes the endowment effect,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45(2009): 947-951

This article is prepared by Appleseed Capital for informational purposes only and is not intended as an offer or solicitation for business. The information and data in this article does not constitute legal, tax, accounting, investment or other professional advice. The views expressed are those of the author(s) as of the date of publication of this report, and are subject to change at any time due to changes in market or economic conditions.
Share This